Anne Klöcker
Institute of Neuroscience, Universitå catholique de Louvain, Belgium
Massimo Penta
Institute of Neuroscience, Universitå catholique de Louvain, Belgium
Vincent Hayward
Institut des Systèmes Intelligents et de Robotique, UPMC Universitå de Paris, France
Jean-Louis Thonnard
Institute of Neuroscience, Universitå catholique de Louvain, Belgium/Cliniques Universitaires Saint-Luc, Physical and Rehabilitation Medicine Department,Universitå catholique de Louvain, Belgium
Download articlePublished in: KEER2014. Proceedings of the 5th Kanesi Engineering and Emotion Research; International Conference; Linköping; Sweden; June 11-13
Linköping Electronic Conference Proceedings 100:88, p. 1041-1066
Published: 2014-06-11
ISBN: 978-91-7519-276-5
ISSN: 1650-3686 (print), 1650-3740 (online)
Humans constantly explore surfaces with their fingertips; providing information regarding the surfaces’ physical attributes and their (un)pleasantness level. It is therefore of interest to investigate whether the perception of pleasantness is related to surfaces’ physical attributes. Pleasant touch perception is generally measured indirectly and generates ordinal scores; lacking fundamental psychometric properties which are essential for objective and quantitative measurement. Consequently; probabilistic measurement models have been established to allow transformations of ordinal scores into linear measures.
Accordingly; we first elaborated a solid basis for future investigations in the domain of pleasantness sensation resulting from active surface explorations with index fingertips. The Rasch model was used to develop a unidimensional; linear and invariant Pleasant Touch Scale; which classifies 37 different everyday life materials according to their pleasantness levels. The latter seemed to be influenced by the respective surfaces’ topographies and by the frictional forces resulting from the tactile surface exploration. These evidences were confirmed in our second study. Indeed; the net values of friction forces; recorded during active fingertip explorations of various material samples of the Pleasant Touch Scale could reliably be correlated with their respective pleasantness measures. A further correlation was found between the fluctuations of friction forces and the surfaces’ pleasantness measures. Our third study was conducted to determine whether (i) these findings hold true for passive fingertip stimulations and (ii) temperature variations of stimuli impact their pleasantness levels. Frictional forces and surfaces’ topographies of stimuli played a crucial role in passive touch pleasantness perception.
Andrich; D. (1978). Application of a psychometric rating model to ordered categories which are scored with
successive integers. Applied Psychological Measurement; 2; 581-594.
Andrich; D.; Sheridan; B.E.; Luo; G. (2004). Rasch unidimensional measurement models (RUMM): A
windows based computer program. Perth; AU: Murdoch University.
Bond; T.G.; Fox; C.M. (2001). Applying The Rasch Model: Fundamental Measurement in the Human
Sciences. New Jersey; NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Ekman; G.; Hosman; J.; Lindstroem; B. (1965). Roughness; Smoothness; and Preference: A Study of
Quantitative Relations in Individual Subjects. J Exp Psychol 70; 18-26.
Essick; G.K.; McGlone; F.; Dancer; C.; Fabricant; D.; Ragin; Y.; Phillips; N.; Jones; T.; Guest; S. (2010).
Quantitative assessment of pleasant touch. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 34; 192-203.
Gibson; J.J. (1962). Observations on active touch. Psychol Rev 69; 477-491.
Guest; S.; Essick; G.; Dessirier; J.M.; Blot; K.; Lopetcharat; K.; McGlone; F. (2009). Sensory and affective
judgments of skin during inter- and intrapersonal touch. Acta Psychol (Amst) 130; 115-126.
Horiuchi; K.; Kashimoto; A.; Tsuchiya; R.; Yokoyama; M. Nakano; K. (2009). Relationship between tactile
sensation and friction signals in cosmetic foundation. Tribology Letters 36; 113-123.
Heller; M.A. (1984). Active and passive touch: the influence of exploration time on form recognition. J Gen
Psychol 110; 243-249.
Johansson; R.S.; Trulsson; M.; Olsson; K.A.; Westberg; K.G. (1988). Mechanoreceptor activity from the
human face and oral mucosa. Exp Brain Res 72: 204-208.
Kandel; E.R.; Schwartz; J.H.; Jessel; T.M. (2000). Principles of Neural Science (4th ed.). New York; NY:
McGraw Hill.
Klöcker; A.; Arnould; C.; Penta; M.; Thonnard; J.L. (2012). Rasch-Built Measure of Pleasant Touch through
Active Fingertip Exploration. Front Neurorobot 6; 1-9.
Klöcker; A.; Wiertlewski; M.; Theate; V.; Hayward; V.; Thonnard; J.L. (2013). Physical Factors Influencing
Pleasant Touch during Tactile Exploration. PLoS One 8; e79085.
Klöcker; A.; Oddo; C.M.; Camboni; D.; Penta; M.; Thonnard; J.L. (2014). Physical factors influencing
pleasant touch during passive fingertip stimulation. Under review.
Landrigan; D.T; Forsyth; G.A. (1974). Regulation and production of movement effects in
exploration-recognition performance. J Exp Psychol 103; 1124-1130.
Lederman; S. (1981). The perception of surface roughness by active and passive touch. Bulletin of the
Psychonomic Society 18; 253-255.
Major; D.R. (1895). On the affective tone of simple sense impressions. American Journal of Psychology 7;
57-77.
McGlone; F.; Reilly; D. (2010). The cutaneous sensory system. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 34: 148-159.
McGlone; F.; Vallbo; A.B.; Olausson; H.; Loken; L.; Wessberg; J. (2007). Discriminative touch and emotional
touch. Can J Exp Psychol 6: 173-183.
Nordin; M. (1990). Low-threshold mechanoreceptive and nociceptive units with unmyelinated (C) fibres in the
human supraorbital nerve. J Physiol 426: 229-240.
Rasch; G. (1960). Probabilistic models for some intelligence and attainment tests. Chicago; IL: Mesa Press.
Richardson; B.R.; Wuillemin; D.B.; Mackintosh; G.J. (1981). Can passive touch be better than active touch? A comparison of active and passive tactile maze learning. British Journal of Psychology 72; 353-362.
Ripin; R.; Lazarsfeld; P.F. (1937). The tactile-kinaesthetic perception of fabrics with emphasis on their
relative pleasantness. Journal of Applied Psychology 21; 198–224.
Smith; A.M.; Chapman; C.E.; Donati; F.; Fortier-Poisson; P.; Hayward; V. (2009). Perception of simulated
local shapes using active and passive touch. J Neurophysiol 102; 3519-3529.
Spence; C.; Gallace; A. (2011). Multisensory Design: Reaching Out to Touch the Consumer. Psychology& Marketing 28; 267-308.
Stevens; S.S. (1975). Psychophysics: Introduction to its perceptual; neural; and social prospects. New
Jersey;NJ: John Wiley & Sons.
Tennant; A.; Conaghan; P.G. (2007). The Rasch measurement model in rheumatology: what is it and why
use it? When should it be applied; and what should one look for in a Rasch paper? Arthritis Rheum 57;
1358-1362.
Tennant; A.; Mckenna; S.P.; Hagell; P. (2004). Application of Rasch analysis in the development and
application of quality of life instruments. Value Health 7 Suppl 1; S22-26.
Tennant; A. (2004 ). Disordered Thresholds: An example from the Functional Independence Measure.
Rasch Measurement Transactions 17; 945-948.
Vega-Bermudez; F.; Johnson; K.O.; Hsiao; S.S. (1991). Human tactile pattern recognition: active versus
passive touch; velocity effects; and patterns of confusion. J Neurophysiol; 65; 531-546.
Wiertlewski; M.; Lozada; J.; Hayward; V. (2011). The Spatial Spectrum Of Tangential Skin Displacement Can
Encode Tactual Texture. Transactions on Robotics 27; 461-472.
Wills; C.E.; Moore; C.F. (1994). A controversy in scaling of subjective states: magnitude estimation versus
category rating methods. Res Nurs Health 17; 231-237.
Wright; B.D.; Masters; G.N. (1982). Rating Scale Analysis. Rasch Measurement. Chicago; IL: Mesa Press.
Wright; B.D.; Linacre; J.M. (1989). Observations are always ordinal; measurements; however; must be
interval. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 70; 857-860.
Wright; B.D.; Stone; M.H. (1979). Best Test Design. Rasch Measurement. Chicago; IL: Mesa Press.